Current
Issues
Taylor (1990) 93% of patients fail to adhere to some aspect of their treatment. Sarafino(1994) People adhere to treatment regimes reasonably closely 78% of the time. Sarafino found the average adherence rates for taking medicine to prevent illness is 60% for short and long term regimes. Compliance to change one's diet or to give up smoking is variable and low.
If you go to the doctors and he or she prescribes tablets that are then not taken, the problem of non-compliance will be exacerbated by the patient lying to the doctor. The doctor will feel that his original prescribed treatment was incorrect and might then prescribe an inappropriate treatment instead.
Compliance with chemotherapy is very high among adults with estimates of better than 90 percent of patients complying with the treatment.
Non compliance takes many forms. Some patients do not keep appointments; others do not follow advice. Many patients fail to collect their prescriptions, discontinue medication early, fail to change their daily routine, and miss follow-up appointments (Sackett and Hayes, 1976).
There are financial
implications for non-compliance. In 1980, between $396 and $792 million were
wasted in the USA because of non-compliance to prescribed drugs (Ogden, 1996).
Methodological problem of estimating the level of adherence to medical regimes.
Percentages are overestimated because patients who tend to volunteer for these studies would be more likely to be compliant.
Patients often lie about their level of adherence, so as to present a good impression of themselves. It has been reported in the press that those patients who smoke may be afforded a low level of priority, when they are in need of a transplant. Patients might lie about their smoking, to avoid such discrimination.
If multiple readings are taken by using several of the methods that check compliance then a more accurate picture of the patients' compliance can be made. If a patient is shown to be non-compliant by several different measures then we can be almost certain that the subject really has not complied.
Patients are less likely to change habits than heed medical advice to take medicine (Haynes, 1976). Patients who view their illness as severe are more likely to comply (Becker & Rosenstock, 1984). Notice it is how the patient views the seriousness of the illness, not what the physician thinks! Doctors tend to blame their patients for non-adherence, attributing their behaviour to characteristics of their patients (mental capacity or personality traits) - Davis (1966). [Can you think how this relates to the Nisbett (Actor-observer effects key study?]. Research has shown that it is not the patient's personality that predicts non-adherence, but a combination of factors arising out of the doctor - patient relationship (e.g. Ley 1982). Factors such as age and gender are predictive of compliance, depending upon what instructions are to be complied with. For example, adolescents are less likely to comply with a long term treatment that makes them appear different from their peers. Classic experiments - Milgram (1963) and Asch (1955. Milgram's experiment demonstrated that ordinary people will obey authority figures, to the extent that they would administer potentially lethal 'electric shocks' to a mild-mannered victim. Asch's experiment demonstrated that people will agree with others even though it is obvious others are wrong.
If medication is prescribed over a long time, it's more likely to be discontinued early (Haynes 1976).
Some patients are complainers and others accept advice more readily. Complainers will always complain and thus be dissatisfied with the doctor, and therefore less compliant. Some patients are predisposed never to follow advice. However there is little evidence for the link between personality and compliance.
Economic factors cannot be excluded; doctors must be sensitive to costs, in terms of both money and time off work (Heib and Wang, 1974).
If several factors are considered, then more accurate predictions of compliance can be forecast. Korsch et al (1978) were able to identify almost 90 percent of non-compliant patients using multiple background variables.
The relationship between patient satisfaction and compliance has been studied by Francis et al. 1969
Patients' Report |
Percent Highly Compliant |
Doctor businesslike |
31 |
Doctor friendly and not businesslike |
46 |
High satisfaction with consultation |
53 |
Moderate satisfaction with consultation |
43 |
Moderate dissatisfaction with consultation |
32 |
High dissatisfaction with consultation |
17 |
Many patients have serious misconceptions about their illness. For example, a stomach ulcer patient who believes that acid is produced in the stomach each time one eats, is less likely to follow the doctor's advice to eat small, frequent meals.
People's attitudes do not necessarily match their behaviour.
Ley model of
patient compliance (1989).
Evaluation
A useful theory or
model in health psychology should:
Ley's model has
satisfied or of the above criteria, as the following research indicates.
Patient
satisfaction
Ley (1988) reviews
21 studies of hospital patients and found that 28% of general practice patients
in the UK were dissatisfied with the treatment they received. Dissatisfaction
amongst hospital patients was even higher with 41 per cent dissatisfied with
their treatment. The dissatisfaction stemmed from affective aspects of the
consultation (e.g. lack of emotional support and understanding), behavioural
aspects (e.g. prescribing, adequate explanations) and competence (e.g.
appropriateness of the referral, diagnosis).
It was found that
patients were "information seekers" (i.e. wanted to know as much
information is possible about their condition), rather than "information
blunters" (i.e. did not want to know the true seriousness of their
condition).
Over 85% of cancer
patients wanted all information about diagnosis, treatment and prognosis (the
chances of treatment being successful) (Reynolds et al., 1981).
60 to 98% of
terminally ill patients wanted to know their bad news (Veatch, 1978).
Older research had
found that a small but significant group did not want to be given the truth for
cancer and heart disease (Kubler-Ross, 1969). These findings could be due, in
part, to the attitudes that prevailed during the late Sixties. Research
suggests that attitudes have changed since then.
Patient understanding
Boyle (1970) asked
patients to define a range of different illnesses and found the following:
Illness to be defined |
% correct |
Arthritis |
85 |
Bronchitis |
80 |
Jaundice |
77 |
Palpitations |
52 |
Patients were also
poor at being able to locate various organs.
Organ |
% correct |
Liver |
49 |
Heart |
42 |
Stomach |
20 |
Roth (1979) found
that although patients understood that smoking is causally related to lung
cancer, 50% thoughts that lung cancer caused by smoking had a good prognosis
for recovery.
It was also found
that 13% of patients thought that hypertension could be cured by treatment when
it can only be managed.
Patient recall
Bain (1977) tested
recall of a sample of patients who attended a GP practice. The following was
found:
Instruction to be recalled |
% unable to recall |
The name of the drug prescribed |
37 |
The frequency of the dose |
23 |
The duration of the treatment |
25 |
Crichton et al.
(1978) found that 22% of patients had forgotten their advised treatment regimes
after visiting their GPs.
Ley (1989) found
that the following factors increased recall of information:
Age has no effect
on recall success.
Compare
this list with the one below
Cognitive and emotional factors in patients' recall of information (DiMatteo & DiNicola 1982).
Homedes (1991) has
reported that more than 200 variables affect compliance. He categorises them
as:
Becker and Rosenstock (1984)
Perceptions of severity and susceptibility by the patient are related to compliance (Becker 1976). Patients who believe they are likely to become ill and that this eventuality would have negative consequences are more likely to take some action. Simple beliefs regarding the likelihood that medication will improve the patient's condition are very potent determinants of compliance (Becker 1976). Any question of safety of treatment, side effects, or distress associated with treatment become very powerful suppressers and reduce the likelihood that patients will do as they were told (Becker 1974).
Actual severity of an illness is not related to compliance, but patient perception of severity is.
Abraham et al (1992) studied 300 sexually active Scottish teenagers. The seriousness of AIDS and the perceived vulnerability of contracting the illness were not the factors that influenced the teenagers. The awkwardness of use and the likely response from their partner, were seen as costs that outweighed the benefits. The teenagers therefore tended not to use condoms! It would make sense to concentrate advertising campaigns on the barriers to condom use.
It is difficult to assess the health belief model as it is difficult to measure variables such as perceived susceptibility. Habits, such as cleaning your teeth are not easily explained by the model. The model has limited predictive value, but can be useful when trying to explain somebody's behaviour.
The Health Belief model is a comprehensive model. Revisions in the model have expanded its range to include intentions as well as beliefs (Becker 1974).Other models that are less comprehensive are the theory of reasoned action, protection motivation theory, Naive health theories and subjective expected utility theory.
Naive health theories.
Patients often develop their own incorrect theories about their illnesses. Such theories develop because a particular behaviour has become erroneously associated with an improvement in their condition. Such beliefs interfere with the understanding of the doctor's instructions. The instructions are interpreted so as to accord with their naive health theory (Bishop and Converse, 1986).
The model has two strengths. One is that it explains why a patient who intends to comply actually does not. Secondly, the model is easily testable.
Sometimes the side effects of a treatment can be so devastating, that the patient decides, quite rationally, not to proceed with the treatment. Bulpitt (1988) medication used for the treatment of hypertension reduced the symptoms of depression and headache. However, the men taking the drug experienced increased sexual problems (difficulty with ejaculation and impotence). Chapin (1980) suggested that 10% of admissions to a geriatric unit were the result of drug side effects. Most non-adherence in arthritis patients was owing to unintentional reasons (e.g. forgetting); the common intentional reasons were side effects and cost (Lorish et al, 1989).
It is better to have a self-efficacy that is slightly higher than one's true ability. This will give one the confidence to undertake a task and make them resilient to giving up. We assess our self-efficacy by:
Pancreas produces the hormone insulin, which is responsible for the storage and use of glucose. Diabetics either produce no insulin (Type I), or do not produce enough insulin (Type II). Type I would require insulin replacement by injection. Type II requires a good diet, weight management and oral medication. Too little glucose in the blood would lead to a hypoglycemic attack ('a hypo'), which is life threatening. Too much glucose produces little immediate effect (unless the patient has too much glucose over a long period of time). The long term effects of too much glucose is likely to be circulation and heart problems (Bradley 1994).
Diabetics have to respond to the following health requests:
Unhygienic injections |
80% |
Wrong insulin dose |
58% |
Inappropriate diet |
75% |
Irregular diet |
75% |
Glasgow et al (1987) - Diabetics have more difficulty following exercise and diet advice. Data was collected by the patients own 'self-report'. This method is unreliable. Glucose-testing machines fitted with a memory chip showed that patients were inaccurate in their self-reporting.
Possible reasons as to why diabetics do not comply might be:
Swigonski (1987) found that social support in kidney disease patients can have adverse effect. Such patients were more likely not to comply with limiting their fluid intake. This is because social occasions often involve drinking!
Bradley (1994) found evidence to support the view that patients can be aware of their glucose levels:
Shillitoe and Miles (1989) in defence of the diabetics against unjust criticism:
'Compliance' seems to be the wrong word. Perhaps 'adherence' or 'levels of self-care behaviour' might be more appropriate, depending on the situation.
Kaplan et al (1993)
Provide more information about the drugs and the treatment.
Tailored regimens are easier to comply with, and there has been some encouraging results (e.g. Haynes et al, 1979).
Get the doctor to improve communication and to be warm and sensitive.
Get the patients to ask more questions so that they get more information. This will help to prevent false beliefs. It also improves patient satisfaction with the doctor, and thus compliance. Some patients may not know what to do if they miss taking some tablets. Others may not know when they could expect to feel better.
Three different types of leaflets were given to patients. One was easy to understand, another was moderately difficult and the other was very difficult to understand. Compliance increased for those patients who were given easy or moderately difficult leaflets (Ley et al 1976).
Just informing patients about their illnesses will not necessarily increase compliance. Patients suffering from hypertension viewed a slide show and read a booklet about hypertension and its treatment, emphasising the benefits of treatment and regular medication. Despite this greater understanding there were no differences in compliance or effectiveness of blood pressure control between patients who had and had not been given the educational program. However, programs that provide information about the problems people will face in trying to adhere to a regimen and focus on changing peoples interpretation of their relapses have been more successful in promoting compliance (Belisle et al, 1987).
Increase the frequency of visits to the doctors. This would increase positive reinforcement. The patient would comply because they would not wish to be scolded by the doctor. Some studies have found compliance to increase by up to 60 percent (e.g. McKenny et al., 1973). When supervision is reduced, so is compliance. For example, non-compliant hypertensive patients were asked to keep a record of their blood pressure and compliance was increased as a result. This only happened when the records were checked on a regular basis.
Nessman et al (1980) group sessions are more effective. In this experiment the treatment was for hypertension (high blood pressure). Compliance increased from 38% to 88%. The result could be misleading because the researchers were only able to persuade 56 people out of a possible 500, to take part! They could have been highly motivated from the start!
(Sarafino 1994)
This will help for short-term regimens more than long-term (Haynes, 1982).
Kulik and Carlino (1987) found that by getting children to promise to take the medicine, significantly more children complied as ascertained by urine samples and interviews with parents. Control children were told to take the medicine, but the doctor did not ask the child to promise. The children were randomly allocated to the two groups. The children were followed up 10 days later.
The health worker has a central role. They can tailor the message to the individual lifestyle of the patient. Face to face contact will increase co-operation. Can get other family members to help out. Progress can be monitored.
Major advantage is that patients can become involved in their treatment (Turk & Meichenbaum, 1991).
Advice to parents of children
with AIDS
Encountering
the health care system - click here for relevant lecture notes
The Compliance and Adherence Process in the Transplant
Patient: Professional Responsibility Bonnie Siegal, PhD, NASW Diplomate
Louise
Ellerby-Jones with some tips for teachers and students