Measuring pain

Summary

Karoly (1985) - we should focus on all of the factors that contribute to pain

1.      Sensory - intensity, duration, threshold, tolerance, location, etc

2.      Neurophysiological - brainwave activity, heart rate, etc

3.      Emotional and motivational - anxiety, anger, depression, resentment, etc

4.      Behavioural - avoidance of exercise, pain complaints, etc

5.      Impact on lifestyle - marital distress, changes in sexual behaviour

6.      Information processing - problem solving skills, coping styles, health beliefs

Techniques used to collect data.

1.      interviews - advantage - it can cover Karoly's 6 points

2.      behavioural observations

3.      psychometric measures

4.      medical records

5.       physiological measures

Physiological measures of pain

Muscle tension is associated with painful condi­tions such as headaches and lower backache, and it can be measured using an electromyograph (EMG). This apparatus measures electrical activity in the muscles, which is a sign of how tense they are. Some link has been established between headaches and EMG patterns, but EMG recordings do not gen­erally correlate with pain perception (Chapman et al 1985) and EMG measurements have not been shown to be a useful way of measuring pain.

Another approach has been to relate pain to autonomic arousal. By taking measures of pulse rate, skin conductance and skin temperature, it may be possible to measure the physiological arousal caused by experiencing pain. Finally, since pain is perceived within the brain, it may he possible to measure brain activity, using an electroencephalograph (EEG), in order to deter­mine the extent to which an individual is experienc­ing pain. It has been shown that subjective reports of pain do correlate with electrical changes that show up as peaks in EEG recordings. Moreover, when analgesics are given, both pain report and waveform amplitude on the EEG are decreased (Chapman et al, 1985).

Evaluation

The advantage of the physiological measures of pain described above is that they are objective (that is, not subject to bias by the person whose pain is being meas­ured, or by the person measuring the pain). On the other hand, they involve the use of expensive machinery and trained personnel. Their main disadvantage, however, is that they are not valid (that is, they do not measure what they say they are measuring). For example, autonomic arousal can occur in the absence of pain being wired up to a machine may be stressful and can cause a per­son’s heart rate to increase. If someone is very anxious about the process of having his or her pain assessed, or else is worried about the mean­ing of the pain, this will cause physiological changes not necessarily related to the intensity of the pain being experienced.  Autonomic responses can be affected by many other factors such as diet, alcohol consumption and infection. E.g. infection present can get increased pulse rate. Better used as a signal for the presence of pain rather than as a direct indices of pain.

 

Observations of pain behaviours

People tend to behave in certain ways when they are in pain; observing such behaviour could provide a means of assessing pain.

Turk, Wack and Kerns (1985) have provided a classification of observable pain behaviours.

 

   Facial /audible expression of distress: grimac­ing and teeth clenching; moaning and sighing.

   Distorted ambulation or posture: limping or walking with a stoop; moving slowly or carefully to protect an injury; supporting, rubbing or hold­ing a painful spot; frequently shifting position.

   Negative affect: feeling irritable; asking for help in walking, or to be excused from activities; asking questions like ‘Why did this happen to me?’

   Avoidance of activity: lying down frequently; avoiding physical activity; using a prosthetic device.

 

One way to assess pain behaviours is to observe them in a clinical setting (although pain is also assessed in a natural setting as the patient goes about his or her everyday activities). Keefe and Williams (1992) have identified five elements that need to be consid­ered when preparing to assess any form of behav­iour through this type of observation.

 

   A rationale for observation: it is important for clinicians to know why they are observing pain behaviours. One reason is to identify ‘problem’ behaviours that the patient may be reluctant to report, such as pain when swallowing, so that treatment can be given. Another is to monitor the progress of a course of treatment.

   A method for sampling pain behaviour techniques for sampling and recording behaviour include continuous observation, measuring dura­tion (how long the patient takes to complete a task), frequency counts (the number of times a target behaviour occurs) and time sampling (for example, observing the patient for five minutes every hour).

   Definitions of the behaviour: observers need to be completely clear as to what behaviours they are looking for.

   Observer training: in most clinical situations, there will be different observers at different times and it is important that they are consistent.

   Reliability and validity: the most useful meas­ure of consistency in observation methods is inter-rater reliability, but test-retest reliability can also be useful. Three types of validity that could be assessed are: concurrent validity (are the results of the observation consistent with another measure of the same behaviour?), construct valid­ity (are the behaviours being recorded really signs of pain?) and discriminant validity (do the obser­vation records discriminate between patients with and without pain?).

 

A commonly used example of an observation tool for, assessing pain behaviour is the UAB Pain Behaviour Scale designed by Richards et al (1982). This scale consists of ten target behaviours and observers have to rate how frequently each occurs. The UAB is easy to use and quick to score; it has scored well on inter-rater and test-retest reliability.

However, correlation between scores on the UAB and on the McGill Pain Questionnaire is low indicating that the relationship between observable pain behaviour and the self-reports of the subjective experience of pain is not a close one. Turk et al (1983) describe techniques that some­one living with the patient (the observer) can use to provide a record of their pain behaviour. These in­clude asking the observer to keep a pain diary, which includes a record of when the patient is in pain and for how long, how the observer recognized the pain, what the observer thought and felt at the time, and how the observer attempted to help the patient alleviate the pain. Other techniques are to interview the observer, or to ask the observer to complete a questionnaire containing questions about how much the pain inter­feres with the patient’s normal activities and social life, the effect of the pain on family relationships and on the moods of both patient and observer.

Commentary

    Behavioural assessment is less objective than taking physiological measurements, because it relies on the observer’s interpretation of the patient’s pain behaviours (although, in practice, this can be partly dealt with by using clearly defined checklists of be­haviour and carrying out inter-rater reliability that is, using two independent observers and comparing their findings).

    An individual may be displaying a great deal of pain behaviour, not because that indi­vidual is in severe pain but because he or she is receiving social reinforcement for the pain behaviour (for example, attention, sympathy and time off work). A by Gil et al (1988) provides an example of this: the children whose pain behaviour (scratching their eczema) was rewarded with attention exhibited more of this behaviour.

Self-report measures

Because pain is a subjective, internal experience, the assessment of pain is therefore best carried out by using patient self-reports, and this is by far the most frequently used technique.

Carroll (1993a) lists the different dimensions of pain that sufferers can be questioned about:

 

    Site of  pain: where is the pain?

    Type of pain: what does the pain feel like?

    Frequency of pain: how often does the pain occur?

    Aggravating or relieving factors: what makes the pain better or worse?

    Disability: how does the pain affect the patient’s everyday life?

    Duration of pain: how long has the pain been present?

    Response to current and previous treatments: how effective have drugs and other treatments been?

 

An important item to add to this list is the emotional and cognitive effect of the pain—in other words, how does the pain make patients feel and how does it affect their thought processes and attitudes?

 

Two types of self-rating scales

1. Visual analogue

fig06Patients mark a continuum of severity from "No Pain" to "Very Severe Pain"

Simple and Quick to use and can be filled out repeatedly

Can track the pain experience as it changes - this could reveal patterns such as situations or times of the day when the pain is better or worse

This method has adequate reliability, however limits pain to a single dimension.  Downie and colleagues evaluated the degree of agreement between various scales in patients with rheumatic diseases and found a high correlation among the different types of scales. The scales are simple to understand and do not demand a high degree of literacy or sophistication on the part of the patient, unlike other pain measurement tools, such as the semantic differential scales described below. The Visual Analogue Scale is simple and quick to administer, and may be used before, during, and following treatment to evaluate changes in the patient's perception of pain relative to treatment. The scales may also be completed throughout the course of a day to assess change in pain intensity relative to activity or time of day.

2. McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ)

The McGill Pain Questionnaire, developed by Melzack (1975), was the first proper self-report pain-measuring instrument and is still the most widely used today.

An attempt to find words to describe experiences of pain was made in a study by Melzack and Torg­erson (1971) in which they asked doctors and uni­versity graduates to classify 102 adjectives into groups describing different aspects of pain. As a result of this exercise, they identified three major psychological dimensions of pain:

 

    sensory: what the pain feels like physically where it is located, how intense it is, its duration and its quality (for example, ‘burning’, ‘throbbing’)

    affective: what the pain feels like emotionally whether it is frightening, worrying and so on

    evaluative: what the subjective overall intensity of the pain experience is (for example, ‘unbear­able’, ‘distressing’).

 

Each of the three main classes was divided into a number of sub-classes (sixteen in total). For ex­ample, the affective class was sub-divided into tension (including the adjectives ‘tiring’, ‘exhaust­ing’), autonomic (including ‘sickening’, ‘suffocating’) and fear (including ‘fearful’, ‘frightful’, ‘terrifying’).

Melzack and Torgerson (1971) then asked a sam­ple of doctors, patients and students to rate the words in each sub-class for intensity. The first 20 questions on the McGill Pain Questionnaire consist of adjectives set out within their sub-classes, in order of intensity. Questions 1 to 10 are sensory, 11 to 15 affective, 16 is evaluative and 17 to 20 are mis­cellaneous.

Patients are asked to tick the word in each sub­class that best describes their pain. Based on this, a pain rating index (PRJ) is calculated: each sub-class is effectively a verbal rating scale and is scored accordingly (that is, 1 for the adjective describing least intensity, 2 for the next one and so on). Scores are given for the different classes (sensory, affective, evaluative and miscellaneous), and also a total score for all the sub-classes. In addition, patients are asked to indicate the location of the pain on a body chart (using the codes E for pain on the surface of the body, I for internal pain and El for both external and internal), and to indicate present pain intensity (PPJ) on a 6-point verbal rating scale. Finally, patients complete a set of three verbal rating scales describing the pattern of the pain.

 Criticism of this questionnaire centres on the need to have extensive understanding of the English language eg discriminate between words such as "Smarting" and "Stinging"

Semantic differential scales, such as the McGill, are difficult and time consuming to complete and demand a sophisticated literacy level, a sufficient attention span, and a normal cognitive state. They therefore are less convenient to use in the clinical environment, but have value when a more detailed analysis of a patient's perception of pain is needed, as in a pain clinic or clinical research setting.

The issue of reliability has been addressed in numerous reports, particularly as it concerns the VAS and the McGill Pain Questionnaire. These reports do not lead to a consensus on reliability of these measurements. They suggest that reliability varies based on the patient groups that were examined for pain. Reliability therefore becomes an issue of "reliable in whose hands?" Reliability of many of the pain measurement methods have not extended in any realistic way beyond the reliability found by the original authors of the pain measurement methods.

A lack of clear reliability information should not prevent the clinician from using these methods, but it should alert the clinician to the possibility that a particular method may not be reliable with a particular patient or a group of patients. The clinician also should ensure that those who use the measurements for their own purposes will be aware of the limitations of these measurements.37

A difficult aspect of reliability is that the patient may have developed a different understanding of the pain problem and may give a different response from one examination to the next. It is equally important for the examiner to ask himself or herself whether the interpretation of the patient's responses differs from one examination to the next. Both factors affect the reliability of the information being gathered.37

Perhaps it is worthwhile to reexamine the concepts of subjective and objective measurements. Sometimes the terms "objective" and "subjective" are concerned not with the reliability of a measurement, but with the nature of what is being measured. It could be argued that pain is a subjective phenomenon, but if it is measured reliably, the quality of the measurement would be objective.

Acknowledgements

Philippe Harari and Karen Legge (2001), Psychology and Health, Heinemann, 0-435-80659-9.  Highly recommended, easy to read, affordable text; a must have for every student.

LINKS

Pain Measurement

Continue with Controlling Pain

Return to Nature and Symptoms of Pain

Summary with blanks to fill in

Return to Gary Sturt's Homepage _